If counting sheep is an abstract concept, or you are unable to visualise the faces of loved ones, you could have aphantasia – a newly-defined condition to describe people who are born without a “mind’s eye”.

Some people report a significant impact on their lives from being unable to visualise memories of their partners or departed relatives. Others say that descriptive writing is meaningless to them, and careers such as architecture or design are closed to them, as they would not be able to visualise an end-product.

Cognitive neurologist Adam Zeman, at the University of Exeter Medical School, has revisited the concept of people who cannot visualise.

It was first identified by Sir Francis Galton in 1880.

A 20th-century survey suggested that this may be true of 2.5 per cent of the population – yet until now, this phenomenon has remained largely unexplored.

Visualisation is the result of activity in a network of regions widely distributed across the brain, working together to enable us to generate images on the basis of our memory of how things look.

These regions include areas in the frontal and parietal lobes, which “organise” the process of visualisation, together with areas in the temporal and occipital lobes, which represent the items we wish to call to the mind’s eye, and give visualisation its “visual” feel.

An inability to visualise could result from an alteration of function at several points in this network. This problem has been described previously following major brain damage and in the context of mood disorder.

Recent research came about when 21 people contacted Zeman after reading an article on his previous research and realising they had never been able to imagine.

Now, Zeman and his team are conducting further studies with those affected to find out more about why some people are born with poor or diminished visual imagery ability.

“This intriguing variation in human experience has received little attention,” he said.

“Our participants mostly have some first-hand knowledge of imagery through their dreams.

“Our study revealed an interesting dissociation between voluntary imagery, which is absent or much reduced in these individuals, and involuntary imagery, for example in dreams, which is usually preserved.”

Zeman and colleagues describe these patients’ experience in a paper published in the journal Cortex.

Sign up to our free newsletters

Get the best updates straight to your inbox:
Please select at least one mailing list.

You can unsubscribe at any time by clicking the link in the footer of our emails. We use Mailchimp as our marketing platform. By subscribing, you acknowledge that your information will be transferred to Mailchimp for processing.